Publication

Research Article

International Journal of MS Care

Q3 | Volume 27

Apathy in Early Relapsing-Remitting Multiple Sclerosis: Prevalence and Predictors

From the Department of Neurology, Hospital da Luz Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal. Correspondence: Inês Brás Marques, MD, Hospital da Luz Lisboa, Avenida Lusíada 100, 1500-650 Lisbon, Portugal; email: inesmbmarques@gmail.com.

Abstract

Background: Apathy, a neuropsychiatric symptom characterized by a lack of motivation and affecting cognitive, emotional, and behavioral domains, is being increasingly recognized in multiple sclerosis (MS), with an estimated prevalence of 19% to 43%. However, apathy remains underinvestigated in early MS. In this study, we aim to evaluate apathy in people with relapsing-remitting MS (RRMS) whose disease duration has been less than 5 years.

Methods: The study included 38 people with RRMS with a disease duration of less than 5 years and 38 matched controls. Clinical and comprehensive neuropsychological evaluations were performed. Apathy was evaluated using the Apathy Evaluation Scale, Clinician Version (AES-C).
Depressive symptoms, anxiety, fatigue, and quality of life were also assessed.

Results: Apathy prevalence was 42.1%. Apathy scores correlated with depressive symptoms, anxiety, fatigue, lower quality of life, younger age, and worse performance in frontal lobe tests (Trail Making Test-A [TMT-A] and Symbol Digit Modalities Test). Independent apathy predictors were depressive symptoms (OR, 4.05; 95% CI, 1.22-13.5; P = .023), global quality of life (OR, 1.7; 95% CI, 1.08-2.64; P = .021), and TMT-A score (OR, 1.14; 95% CI, 1.02-1.27; P = .020).

Conclusions: Apathy is a disabling symptom that is present in a significant percentage of people in the first years after they have been diagnosed with RRMS. Thus, people with MS should should be routinely screened for apathy, and symptoms should be managed when it occurs. We identified depressive symptoms, low quality of life, and executive dysfunction as predictors of apathy in this population.

Practice Points
  • Apathy is already present in many patients
    within the first years after their diagnosis with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (MS).
  • Apathy correlates with depressive symptoms, lower quality of life, and worse executive function.
  • Routine assessment of apathy should be integrated into the evaluation of patients recently diagnosed with MS.

Apathy is a behavioral and psychological syndrome characterized by a lack of motivation that impacts cognitive, emotional, or behavioral domains.1,2 Apathy is defined as a quantitative reduction of goal-directed activity compared with previous functioning that persists for a minimum of 4 weeks and affects at least 2 of the dimensions of behavior, cognition, emotionality, and social interaction.2 This decline in initiative or interest in usual activities and emotional expression or responsiveness may cause clinically significant functional impairments that cannot be fully explained by other factors, such as physical disability, diminished consciousness, substance effects, and major changes in the patients’ environment.2

Apathy is recognized as a neuropsychiatric manifestation of many neurological disorders, including Parkinson disease, Alzheimer disease, frontotemporal dementia, Huntington disease, cerebrovascular disease, and mild cognitive impairment.3,4 This disabling symptom is also being increasingly recognized in patients with multiple sclerosis (MS), with an estimated prevalence of 19% to 43%.5-13 Some studies in MS have also identified a correlation between apathy and cognitive dysfunction, particularly in executive functions, with apathy severity being a predictor of cognitive dysfunction at follow-up.8,14-17

Despite these findings, apathy is still not part of the routine clinical assessment of people with MS, and its impact on patients’ lives is probably being underestimated. Additionally, studies evaluating apathy in the early stages of MS are lacking in the literature, and its prevalence in these initial phases is still underinvestigated. Thus, this study aims to evaluate the prevalence of apathy in people in the early phase of relapsing-remitting MS (RRMS). The association between apathy and clinical characteristics, cognitive functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and quality of life will also be assessed.

Methods

Participants

This study included a group of people aged 18 to 49 years with RRMS, as defined by the 2017 McDonald Criteria,18 with a disease duration of less than 5 years, who were followed in a neuroimmunology outpatient clinic of a tertiary center. Exclusion criteria included a diagnosis of progressive forms of MS, individuals under 18 or over 50 years of age, steroid treatment in the 4 weeks before the evaluation, a history of severe mental disorder (eg, major depression, psychosis, or schizoaffective disorder), other neurological conditions adversely affecting cognitive functions (eg, dementia, severe traumatic brain injury, stroke), and a history of substance abuse. Healthy controls (HCs) matched to patient demographics, including age, sex, and years of education, were also recruited. The protocol of the research project was approved by the ethics committee of our institution (Comissão de Ética do Hospital da Luz), and it conforms to the provisions of the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to assessments.

Assessments
Clinical Evaluation

All participants underwent a clinical evaluation, including a full medical history and a complete neurological examination. Data collection included demographic variables such as age, sex, and years of education, as well as disease-specific information, including duration of MS, age at MS diagnosis, history of MS relapses, and current pharmacological treatment. Physical disability was assessed using the Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS).19

Apathy Assessment

Apathy was assessed through a clinical interview using the Apathy Evaluation Scale, Clinician Version (AES-C).1 This 18-item clinician-administered scale assesses various aspects of affective, behavioral, and cognitive apathy during the preceding 4 weeks. Each item has 4 possible answers rated from 0, not at all true, to 4, very true, with a total score ranging from 18 to 72. Higher scores indicate more severe apathetic symptoms. Scoring guidelines provided by the original author are based on a clinician’s best judgment of the subject’s thoughts, feelings, and actions, evaluating both verbal and nonverbal data. The AES-C has been validated for the Portuguese population with defined cutoff points.20 In this study, participants with AES-C scores higher than the cutoff scores adjusted for educational level were categorized as apathetic.

Cognitive Evaluation

A comprehensive formal neuropsychological assessment was performed by 2 trained neuropsychologists (CC and DS) with the following battery of tests: Montreal Cognitive Assessment,21 a screening tool for global cognitive functioning; California Verbal Learning test22 for verbal learning and memory; Controlled Oral Word Association Test23 for verbal fluency; Digit Span Test Forward and Digit Span Test Backward24 for immediate attention and working memory; Rey-Osterrieth complex Figure Test25 for visuo-constructional ability, planning, and spatial orientation; Stroop Color-Word Interference test26 for flexibility and inhibitory control; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)27 for information processing speed and attention; Trail Making test Parts A (TMT-A) and B (TMT-B)28 for attention, information processing speed, and set-shifting ability; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)29 for abstract reasoning, monitoring, and cognitive flexibility; and Judgment of Line Orientation test30 for visuospatial perception.

Depression, Anxiety, Fatigue, and Quality-of-Life Assessment

Severity of depressive symptoms and anxiety were evaluated by the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9)31 and the General Anxiety Disorder Assessment (GAD-7),32 respectively. Fatigue was evaluated using the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS)33 and quality of life was assessed with the 12-Item Short Form Survey (SF-12).34

Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were utilized to present absolute and relative frequencies for categorical variables, while continuous variables were summarized using mean with SD or medians, as appropriate. Group comparisons were conducted using an independent-samples t test, Mann-Whitney U test, and χ2 test, depending on the variable type. Correlational analysis was performed using Spearman correlation and 2-tailed Pearson correlation to examine the relationship between apathy and clinical characteristics, cognition, mood disorders, fatigue, or quality of life. Variables found to be significant in the univariate analysis were selected for inclusion in a multivariate logistic regression model to identify predictors of apathy. All reported P values were 2-sided, and statistical significance was considered for values less than .05.

Results

In total, 38 people with MS and 38 matched HCs were included in the study. Table 1 presents participant demographic and clinical data. People with MS had a mean disease duration of 1.9±1.3 years and a median EDSS score of 1.8.

Table 1. Participant Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Table 1. Participant Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

We found a prevalence of apathy in people with MS of 42.1% (n = 16); they had a statistically significant higher score on the AES-C than controls (30 ± 9.69 vs 25.13 ± 5.43; P = .009). Apathy with no significant depressive symptoms associated, or pure apathy, was present in 43.8% (n = 7) of patients with apathy.

Table 2 shows the correlations between AES-C scores and disease characteristics, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and cognitive functions of people with MS.

Table 2. Apathy Correlations

Table 2. Apathy Correlations

In the correlation analysis, AES-C scores exhibited negative correlations with SF-12 scores (r = –0.47; P = .003), SF-12 mental scores (r = –0.61; P<.001), age (r = –0.45; P = .004), and age at diagnosis (r = –0.47; P = .003). Positive correlations were observed between AES-C scores and PHQ-9 scores (r = 0.55; P<.001), GAD-7 scores (r = 0.63; P<.001), MFIS total scores (r = 0.43; P = .007), MFIS cognitive scores (r = 0.47; P = .003), and MFIS social scores (r = 0.43; P = .008). No significant correlations were found between apathy and years of education, disease duration, or physical disability.

For cognitive functions, a correlation was found between apathy and performance on the TMT-A (r = 0.33; P = .042) and the SDMT (r = –0.34; P = .036).

In the multivariate analysis, PHQ-9 scores (OR, 4.05; 95% CI, 1.22-13.5; P = .023), SF-12 total scores (OR, 1.7; 95% CI, 1.08-2.64; P = .021), SF-12-mental scores (OR, 0.603; 95% CI, 0.39-0.93; P = .022), and TMT-A scores (OR, 1.139; 95% CI, 1.02-1.27; P = .020) were independent predictors of apathy.

Discussion

In this study, we assessed the prevalence of apathy and the correlational and predictive relationship with clinical, neuropsychiatric, and cognitive measures in people with early RRMS. We found the overall prevalence of apathy was 42.1%, consistent with reported prevalence rates in other studies focusing on apathy in MS.5,7-10,12,13,16 To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study evaluating apathy prevalence in early RRMS, suggesting that it occurs early in the disease course in a substantial proportion of patients.

This study revealed a negative correlation between apathy and quality of life, with higher apathy scores associated with lower global and mental quality of life, as also reported in another study in MS.35 Additionally, we identified lower quality of life as an independent predictor of the presence of apathy, which underscores the impact apathy may exert on the lives of people with MS from the earlier stages of the disease.

We also identified a correlation between apathy and depressive symptoms, consistent with other studies,7,13 and depressive symptoms were an independent predictor of apathy.

Although apathy and depression symptoms may overlap, many patients present with pure apathy, as did 43.8% of our population. This supports the findings that apathy can manifest independently of depression, as previously reported.5,6,8,9,17

While symptoms such as loss of interest, lack of initiative, and social withdrawal may be present in both apathy and depression, distinctions exist. In depression, the loss of motivation is usually linked to feelings of hopelessness, sadness, and pessimism.17 Apathy is characterized by difficulties in planning and executing voluntary actions, resulting in decreased goal-oriented behavior in response to external stimuli. This differentiation underscores apathy as a distinct entity from depression, with different underlying pathophysiological mechanisms.17,36-38

Regarding cognitive functions, higher apathy scores correlated with worse performance in frontal lobe tests, namely TMT-A and SDMT, which evaluate attention, information processing speed, and set-shifting ability. Further, a higher TMT-A score emerged as an independent predictor of apathy. These results suggest an association between apathy and executive dysfunction, which has also been reported in other MS studies.8,16,17,39

Abnormalities in frontal-subcortical circuits are proposed to underlie both motor and cognitive impairments that result in apathy, suggesting that apathy might be a behavioral disorder associated with cognitive dysexecutive syndrome.40-42 Studies indicate that apathy may be associated with disruptions in the medial frontal cortex (including the anterior cingulate cortex and orbitofrontal cortex) and subcortical structures (such as the ventral striatum, medial thalamus, and ventral tegmental area).43,44 A recent study of MS13 identified microstructural damage within the frontal cortex, caudate nucleus, and frontostriatal white matter bundles as significant pathological substrates of apathy in MS.

We observed a significant correlation between apathy and fatigue, with elevated apathy scores being associated with higher levels of global, cognitive, and social fatigue. Previous research also reported an association between apathy and fatigue.7,13,45 In addition, our study identified a correlation between apathy and anxiety symptoms, as reported in another study in MS.13 However, apathy is an independent neuropsychiatric syndrome, distinct from depression, fatigue, and anxiety. The co-occurrence of these symptoms in some patients may be explained by overlapping aspects in their pathophysiology3,7,45; further research is needed to fully understand the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying their associations.

Anxiety, cognitive deficits, and apathy often present simultaneously in patients, frequently exacerbating each other and contributing to a complex clinical picture that is challenging to disentangle. Each condition may affect motivation and behavior through distinct mechanisms.

Anxiety disrupts motivation by reallocating cognitive resources to threat-related stimuli, thereby impairing the efficient functioning of the goal-directed attentional system.46,47 Cognitive processes such as attention, working memory, and decision-making are particularly susceptible to the effects of anxiety, leading to avoidance behaviors and a reduced capacity for effective planning and problem-solving.46

Cognitive deficits further exacerbate both anxiety and apathy by undermining executive functions critical for planning, problem-solving, and maintaining goal-oriented focus.48,49 Apathy, in turn, can reinforce cognitive deficits, as it is often associated with disruptions in executive functioning, as previously discussed.

This intricate interplay between anxiety, cognitive deficits, and apathy poses a significant challenge in understanding and addressing their combined impact on motivation and behavior. Effective intervention requires a comprehensive approach that accounts for their overlapping mechanisms and distinct manifestations.

We also observed a correlation between apathy and younger age, which has not been previously reported in the literature, although some studies in MS reported an association between apathy and longer disease duration.8,15 This study evaluated a population of younger people with MS with short disease durations and low physical disability, which may have revealed that a younger age at MS onset could be linked to higher levels of apathy.

Previous studies8,15 have reported an association between apathy and higher disability levels in MS patients. However, our study did not replicate this finding, likely due to the characteristics of our sample, where all patients exhibited low physical disability levels.

The strengths of our study include the comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation, the use of the clinician version of the apathy scale, and the assessment of multiple outcome measures, all of which provide a better understanding of the factors associated with apathy in MS.

The limitations were the relatively small sample size and a select population of young patients with low physical disability, which may limit the generalizability of our findings to all people with early MS. In addition, the study did not explore the relationship between apathy and
disease-modifying treatments; imaging correlates of apathy were also not assessed.

Recognition of apathy in clinical practice is crucial, as this neuropsychiatric symptom can significantly affect disease management. Apathetic individuals may not adhere to appointments, medication regimens, and treatment instructions, alongside a reduced willingness to exert effort in their own care. Moreover, they often display a diminished concern for their health and a decline in social interactions. Thus, apathy is associated with a reduction in quality of life, as well as with depression, anxiety, and cognitive executive dysfunction.

Our results highlight the need to develop rehabilitation strategies to address apathy early in the MS disease course to minimize the significant impact it has on patients’ daily lives and activities and ultimately improve their well-being and quality of life.

A multidisciplinary approach, including rehabilitation programs designed to encourage engagement and activity, cognitive behavioral therapy to encourage positive behavioral changes and structured goal-setting, along with the pharmacological management of comorbid conditions such as depression, anxiety, and fatigue, can be useful in managing apathy and improving overall patient outcomes.

Conclusions

The impact of apathy on various facets of patients’ lives, including social adaptation, quality of life, adherence to therapy, and caregiver distress is likely being underestimated. We identified depressive symptoms, diminished quality of life, and executive dysfunction as predictors of apathy in this population. Early recognition and management of apathy in MS is crucial because a significant percentage of people with RRMS present with apathy in the first years of the disease.

Acknowledgments: The authors acknowledge Renato Oliveira for his assistance in the initial setup of this study.

Conflicts of Interest: The author(s) declare no potential conflicts of interest.

Funding Statement: This project (Reference LH.INV.F2019013) was partially financed by Hospital da Luz Lisboa under the initiative “Luz investigação—Bolsas Luz Saúde.” Hospital da Luz Lisboa was not involved in the scientific aspects of the study or the publication.

Prior Presentation: The content of this work was presented as an e-poster at the 10th Congress of the European Academy of Neurology; June 29-July 2, 2024; Helsinki, Finland.

References

  1. Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S. Reliability and validity of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psychiatry Res. 1991;38(2):143-162. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(91)90040-v

  2. Robert P, Lanctôt KL, Agüera-Ortiz L, et al. Is it time to revise the diagnostic criteria for apathy in brain disorders? the 2018 International Consensus Group. Eur Psychiatry. 2018;54:71-76. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.07.008

  3. Fahed M, Steffens DC. Apathy: neurobiology, assessment and treatment. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci. 2021;19(2):181-189. doi:10.9758/cpn.2021.19.2.181

  4. Husain M, Roiser JP. Neuroscience of apathy and anhedonia: a transdiagnostic approach. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(8):470-484. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0029-9

  5. Raimo S, Trojano L, Spitaleri D, Petretta V, Grossi D, Santangelo G. Apathy in multiple sclerosis: a validation study of the apathy evaluation scale. J Neurol Sci. 2014;347(1-2):295-300. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2014.10.027

  6. Diaz-Olavarrieta C, Cummings JL, Velazquez J, Garcia de la Cadena C. Neuropsychiatric manifestations of multiple sclerosis. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;11(1):51-57. doi:10.1176/jnp.11.1.51

  7. Novo AM, Batista S, Tenente J, et al. Apathy in multiple sclerosis: gender matters. J Clin Neurosci. 2016;33:100-104. doi:10.1016/j.jocn.2016.02.038

  8. Raimo S, Trojano L, Spitaleri D, Petretta V, Grossi D, Santangelo G. The relationships between apathy and executive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology. 2016;30(6):767-774. doi:10.1037/neu0000279

  9. Raimo S, Trojano L, Gaita M, Spitaleri D, Santangelo G. Assessing apathy in multiple sclerosis: validation of the dimensional apathy scale and comparison with apathy evaluation scale. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2020;38:101870. doi:10.1016/j.msard.2019.101870

  10. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Assessing the behavioral consequences of multiple sclerosis: an application of the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe). Cogn Behav Neurol. 2003;16(1):54-67. doi:10.1097/00146965-200303000-00007

  11. Rosti-Otajärvi E, Hämäläinen P. Behavioural symptoms and impairments in multiple sclerosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mult Scler. 2013;19(1):31-45. doi:10.1177/1352458512439437

  12. Figved N, Klevan G, Myhr KM, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with multiple sclerosis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(6):463-468. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00624.x

  13. Tazza F, Schiavi S, Leveraro E, et al. Clinical and radiological correlates of apathy in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2024;30(2):247-256. doi:10.1177/13524585231217918

  14. Figved N, Benedict R, Klevan G, et al. Relationship of cognitive impairment to psychiatric symptoms in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2008;14(8):1084-1090. doi:10.1177/1352458508092262

  15. Niino M, Mifune N, Kohriyama T, et al. Apathy/depression, but not subjective fatigue, is related with cognitive dysfunction in patients with multiple sclerosis. BMC Neurol. 2014;14:3. doi:10.1186/1471-2377-14-3

  16. Raimo S, Spitaleri D, Trojano L, Santangelo G. Apathy as a herald of cognitive changes in multiple sclerosis: a 2-year follow-up study. Mult Scler. 2020;26(3):363-371. doi:10.1177/1352458519828296

  17. Raimo S, Trojano L, Gaita M, d’Onofrio F, Spitaleri D, Santangelo G. Relationship between apathy and cognitive dysfunctions in multiple sclerosis: a 4-year prospective longitudinal study. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2022;63:103929. doi:10.1016/j.msard.2022.103929

  18. Thompson AJ, Banwell BL, Barkhof F, et al. Diagnosis of multiple sclerosis: 2017 revisions of the McDonald criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17(2):162-173. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30470-2

  19. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology. 1983;33(11):1444-1452. doi:10.1212/wnl.33.11.1444

  20. Caeiro L, Silva T, Ferro JM, Pais-Ribeiro J. Metric properties of the Portuguese version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psicologia Saúde Doenças. 2012;13:266-282. doi:10.15309/12psd130209

  21. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695-699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

  22. Elwood RW. The California Verbal Learning Test: psychometric characteristics and clinical application. Neuropsychol Rev. 1995;5(3):173-201. doi:10.1007/BF02214761

  23. Benton AL, Hamsher K, Sivan AB. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. 2nd ed. AJA Associates; 1983.

  24. Jones G, Macken B. Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition. 2015;144:1-13. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009

  25. Shin MS, Park SY, Park SR, Seol SH, Kwon JS. Clinical and empirical applications of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. Nat Protoc. 2006;1(2):892-899. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.115

  26. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643-662. doi:10.1037/h0054651

  27. Ryan J, Woods RL, Britt CJ, et al. Normative data for the Symbol Digit Modalities Test in older White Australians and Americans, African-Americans, and Hispanic/Latinos. J Alzheimers Dis Rep. 2020;4(1):313-323. doi:10.3233/ADR-200194

  28. Arnett JA, Labovitz SS. Effect of physical layout in performance of the Trail Making Test. Psychol Assess. 1995;7(2):220-221. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.7.2.220

  29. Berg EA. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J Gen Psychol. 1948;39(1):15-22. doi:10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159

  30. Benton AL, Sivan AB, Hamsher KdeS, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual. Oxford University Press; 1983.

  31. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(9):606-613. doi:10.1046/j.1525
    -1497.2001.016009606.x

  32. Swinson RP. The GAD-7 scale was accurate for diagnosing generalised anxiety disorder. Evid Based Med. 2006;11(6):184. doi:10.1136/ebm.11.6.184

  33. Larson RD. Psychometric properties of the modified fatigue impact scale. Int J MS Care. 2013;15(1):15-20. doi:10.7224/1537-2073.2012-019

  34. Ware J Jr, Kosinski M, Keller SD. A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey: construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med Care. 1996;34(3):220-233. doi:10.1097/00005650-199603000-00003

  35. Klevan G, Jacobsen CO, Aarseth JH, et al. Health related quality of life in patients recently diagnosed with multiple sclerosis. Acta Neurol Scand. 2014;129(1):21-26. doi:10.1111/ane.12142

  36. Dujardin K. Apathie et pathologies neuro-dégénératives: physiopathologie, évaluation diagnostique et traitement. Rev Neurol (Paris). 2007;163(5):513-521. doi:10.1016 /s0035-3787(07)90458-0

  37. Levy ML, Cummings JL, Fairbanks LA, et al. Apathy is not depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;10(3):314-319. doi:10.1176/jnp.10.3.314

  38. Lanctôt KL, Ismail Z, Bawa KK, et al. Distinguishing apathy from depression: a review differentiating the behavioral, neuroanatomic, and treatment-related aspects of apathy from depression in neurocognitive disorders. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2023;38(2):e5882. doi:10.1002/gps.5882

  39. Raimo S, Gaita M, Costanzo A, Spitaleri D, Santangelo G. Distinct neuropsychological correlates of apathy sub-domains in multiple sclerosis. Brain Sci. 2023;13(3):385. doi:10.3390/brainsci13030385

  40. Santangelo G, Vitale C, Trojano L, et al. Relationship between apathy and cognitive dysfunctions in de novo untreated Parkinson’s disease: a prospective longitudinal study. Eur J Neurol. 2015;22(2):253-260. doi:10.1111/ene.12467

  41. Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol. 1993;50(8):873-880. doi:10.1001/archneur.1993.00540080076020

  42. Stuss DT. Functions of the frontal lobes: relation to executive functions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(5):759-765. doi:10.1017/S1355617711000695

  43. Levy R, Dubois B. Apathy and the functional anatomy of the prefrontal cortex-basal ganglia circuits. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(7):916-928. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj043

  44. Le Heron C, Apps MAJ, Husain M. The anatomy of apathy: a neurocognitive framework for amotivated behaviour. Neuropsychologia. 2018;118(Pt B):54-67. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.003

  45. Daumas L, Corbel C, Zory R, et al. Associations, overlaps and dissociations between apathy and fatigue. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):7387. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-11071-5

  46. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7(2):336-353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

  47. Bishop SJ. Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(7):307-316. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008

  48. Stuss DT, Alexander MP. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol Res. 2000;63(3-4):289-298. doi:10.1007/s004269900007

  49. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Related Videos
Related Content